The Improbable Path to Advanced Intelligence
Astronomers interested in extraterrestrial
intelligence often argue Improbable though the evolution of advanced intelligence
may be, you must admit that there is only one sample, one planetary history and
that intelligence evolved on earth. But this is naïve. Biogeography and
paleontology show that earth provides not one sample, but many millions of
samples. Sketches of a handful of these samples follow.
Placental mammals never made it to Australia.
Instead, the marsupials radiated there into a very wide variety of niches that
remarkably paralleled their mammalian lookalikes elsewhere: marsupial dogs,
cats, moles, flying squirrels, and kangaroo mice. Of course some of the niches
in Australia were filled by unique adaptations. The kangaroo, for example,
fills the grazing/browsing niches occupied by ungulates elsewhere. Not only did
intelligence, or even placentation, fail to evolve among these marsupials (who
after all covered a continent the size of the United States), but a form even
remotely like our primitive primate ancestors also failed to appear. From the
earliest days the homeland of the entire primate lineage has been in the trees,
and it was not as if the tree niche in Australia had not been exploited by
marsupials. The koalas adaptation to tree life makes it a marsupials
nomination for a monkey. If youve ever
held a koala in your arms, you know that its brain and behavior could only be
generously described as somnolent. The other tree-exploiting candidate for
primatehood in Australia is the tree kangaroo. If ever there were a hopeful
monster, this must be it. Its secondary adaptation to tree life was cobbled
together from its hopping adaptation to the ground. So long as there are no
serious predators, the tree kangaroo can make comfortable living in the
branches. But when humans appear it becomes just another easy lunch. The
Australian fauna illustrate a remarkable independent radiation of forms
parallel to the mammals. But nowhere do these marsupials display any
inclination toward advanced intelligence, or even important preconditions for
it to develop.
Consider another example, the island of
Madagascar. Some fifty percent of all primate varieties live on Madagascar, but
they are all prosimians. Madagascar broke away from Africa before monkeys
colonized it, and no monkey or ape has ever emerged on Madagascar.
This does not mean that prosimians were not successful.
Far from it, they radiated into an enormous variety of niches. There are
prosimians adapted to swamps, deserts, mountain forests; they are mostly found
in trees, but some adapted to the ground and all habitats in between. Like
other prosimians in Africa and Asia the prosimians body and brain reflect the
fact that they live in a world in which tactile and olfactory reception is as
important as vision. Most have vibrissae, scent glands, and a basic mammalian
body plan. Some have multiple young and some forage at night. Yet they also
have the primate hallmark: grasping hands. Prosimians include some of the
smallest mammals on earth, and in historic times there were lemurs as large as
a donkey. Despite this rather astonishing radiation of forms and adaptations,
prosimians never evolved what could even be generously called a monkey-level
of intelligence.
The tarsier, a prosimian of Asia, developed
binocular color vision rather like the system that evolved in humans. Why
should this be when other prosimians lack binocular vision. The tarsier is
about the size of a rat; it bounds around in trees in semi-darkness in the
Phillippines and parts of southeast Asia. A unique trait in this group is the
greatly elongated tarsial bone, which gives the tarsiers a great deal of extra
leverage when they decide to hop. If humans had such an adaptation, we could
all leap tall buildings at a single bound. During a long leap a tarsier can
grab a moth in mid-air and still make a safe landing. Such acrobatics require
the precision provided by stereoscopic color vision. It is ironic (but no great
surprise in evolutionary history) that a characteristic like binocular color
vision, that turns out to be so critically important in the development of our
own intelligence and technology, also evolved millions of years ago in an
utterly different context, and in support of a very different evolutionary
path.
The evolution of monkeys provides two parallel
examples. After South America split away from Africa, the independent evolution
of New World monkeys reveals many close parallels with Old World monkeys, but
significant differences as well. South American monkeys have exploited a wide
variety of niches. There are many species of squirrel-sized, pair-bonding (in
fact, often polyandrous) marmosets and tamarins. There are species with
male-pattern baldness and no tails. Provocatively, the ape niche has been
filled in South America by three genera of large monkeys, but in a manner
utterly different from arm-swinging apes. Instead these monkeys developed a
fifth and unique appendage, complete with fingerprints: the grasping tail. Their prehensile tails allow them to
distribute their weight and harvest fruits and other goodies out on the
terminal limbs of trees where the foraging is richest. So, the ape niche was
filled in the South American rainforests, but not by an ape. Indeed, there is
no reason to suppose that a creature the size and intelligence of the great
apes would ever have appeared in South America.
In numbers and variety the Old World monkeys,
found throughout Africa, tropical Asia and southeast Asia, are among the most
successful of all mammalian species. The baboons of Africa and their close
cousins, the macaques of Asia (e.g., the rhesus monkey) are well known to most
people. I began studying baboons in 1959because, quite unlike most monkeys and apes, baboons foraged on the ground in
large multi-male groups, were reasonably good hunters, adroit at escaping
predators, and had a social organization that could support a long period of
infant dependency. In other words these large monkeys show many of the traits
we know became important in hominid evolution. (But such comparisons can be
easily stretched too far; baboon solutions to their terrestrial niche are still
monkey solutions, and quite unlike the way of life, strategies, etc. we know
from human foragers).Baboons seem to have many of the prerequisites for advanced intelligence: large
brains, intense curiosity, and exercise considerable insight and guile in
social interactions. From their long fossil record we also know that there have
been many species of baboons, some quite small, and some gorilla-sized. Despite
their admirable successes - their ingenuity, social complexity, and long lives - as
far back in prehistory as we can trace baboons, they seem to be built to the
same basic plan. From the point of view of developing an ape or human level of
intelligence, one can only say that baboons are mired in a baboon niche.
Contributed by: Dr. Irven DeVore
|